Trinicenter.com Trinicenter.com Trinidad and Tobago News
Online Forums
  Welcome, Guest. Please Login
Trinicenter.com International Forum
  HomeHelpSearchLogin  
 
Evolving by Accident, Not Fitness (Read 2157 times)
World News
Administrator
*****
Offline


Trinicenter

Posts: 313
Gender: male
Evolving by Accident, Not Fitness
Oct 14th, 2003 at 1:35pm
 
By DAVID BERREBY, NY Times
Published: October 14, 2003

USTIN, Tex. — Ambition or love? Freedom or security? Perfect job here or perfect mate in Utah? Life forces painful choices on Dr. Michael J. Ryan's colleagues, but it is even harder on his research subject, the male túngara frog: the more sex it gets, the surer it is to be eaten.

Dr. Ryan, who leads the integrative biology section at the University of Texas, is best known for work on the túngara, but he ponders other animals that live between a rock and a hard place: an all-female species of fish that clones itself but must mate with an alien male to do it; tiny salamanders with cells too big for their body plans; birds that must feed the chicks of another kind of bird to support their offspring.

Biologists should learn from life's existential quandaries, he says, but many do not. Instead, they sweep the difficulties under the rug of "adaptationism," the notion that everything about an animal's body and behavior has been honed to enhance its "fitness" or chance of passing on genes.

That idea reminds him of Dr. Pangloss in Voltaire's satire "Candide," who says all is for the best in this best of all possible worlds — even earthquakes, plagues and torture.

"Our take is not the strict Panglossian approach," Dr. Ryan said, over the nachos and beer that friends correctly predicted would be a part of a Ryan talkfest. "Ultimately, what we want to know is why animals are the way they are. Saying some behavior enhances fitness doesn't tell you why birds signal with songs while coral-reef fishes have flashing colors and mammals use odors."

The túngara, a brown-backed, white-bellied creature scarcely bigger than the last joint of his thumb, made him abandon adaptationist thinking, he says. More than 20 years of work on the frog has shown that adaptive pressure alone did not shape it, he said. Its anatomy and ancestry also played a role.

That emphasis has made Dr. Ryan prominent in an evolutionary debate that extends well beyond the love lives of stoic amphibians and weird fish. After all, humans make mysterious choices, too.

They gather, for instance, in groups of 100,000 to listen to patterns of sound from loudspeakers. If Dr. Ryan and his allies are right, then immediate adaptive value is the wrong place to look for a phenomenon like rock concerts. Music may appeal to brain wiring that was in place for thousands of years before anyone sang a note. In any event, he says, anatomy, not adaptation, is the right place to start.

"If we want to understand music preferences, let's look at what the brain is doing," he said. "Then we can ask, What in history made the brain like this?"

In developing his argument, Dr. Ryan has made the túngara (pronounced TOON-guh-ruh), native to Central America and the Caribbean, into a star. It is now one of biology's "model systems" — animals studied by so many scientists, from so many angles, that knowledge about them snowballs, feeding on itself. When he began working on it, he had it almost to himself.

That was in the mid-1980's, when his dissertation research established that female túngara prefer larger males.

Now, field workers at the Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute in Panama observe frogs doing what comes naturally in their home range. Other scientists track how mating calls turn genes on and off. Still others prepare electrodes to measure how calls affect the living brain. ("I've been talking to people about doing M.R.I.'s, too," Dr. Ryan said, "but you know, these frogs are tiny.")

Then there are other colleagues who have used computers to simulate evolution of the frog's mating call. Still others plumb its evolutionary history, using DNA differences to reconstruct details about extinct frogs, like the shape of an ear or the sound of the call. The research illustrates how biology's once separate branches now intertwine: jungle frogs, lab frogs, frog genes and frog brain cells are linked in a single body of work. "If you want to understand living things, you really have to let all these levels of analysis interact," Dr. Ryan said.

That approach used to be common, but now it bucks the intellectual tide, said Dr. Marc D. Hauser, a psychologist at Harvard who has made use of Dr. Ryan's ideas.

We're living in this age of narrowing and reductionism," he said, "and Mike is one of the few who has kept true to the idea that there are many different questions you can ask about animal behavior."

Son of a truck driver and a secretary, Michael Ryan grew up in Northern New Jersey and went to college thinking he would teach high school. But the pleasures of research caught him, through a master's (Rutgers), Ph.D. (Cornell), postdoctorate (Berkeley) and his post here, where he has taught for 20 years. With his fieldwork tan and his reedy, cheerful voice, Dr. Ryan, 49, is the model of a happy researcher. He has been thinking about biology since a high school teacher, a Benedictine monk, got him excited about science.

Dr. Ryan enjoys every facet of a biologist's life — from abstract theorizing to a jungle night recording amphibian sex acts. One fish study began after he noticed unusual-looking males as he snorkeled a Mexican river. Several major findings emerged from thoughts that popped into his head as he was gabbing about experiments. He even likes writing papers, and he has published about 140.

"I tell my students, writing and explaining is part of life in science, so you'd better find a way to make it fun," he said.

In a small lab area filled with the country-night sounds of chirping crickets (the frog food), Dr. Ryan reaches into a moss-filled container and pulls out a tiny brown túngara, which hops once and then perches on his finger. "She's kind of fat. Probably has a few eggs," he says. "A male would have this big vocal sac to make his calls."

That call is the problem for a male túngara. A sliding whine followed by abrupt chucks, it sounds a bit like a little boy imitating a dive bomber. Female frogs hop to when they hear it. But hungry fringe-lipped bats also tune in; the call is their beacon for finding frogs to gobble.

So a male túngara is caught between two evolutionary pressures. And as Darwin himself pointed out, sexual selection creates paradoxical traits in males of many species, like huge peacock tails — so useless for flight, so good for impressing peahens.

To a biologist who thinks traits serve fitness here and now, all this is just natural selection with an extra step. In the case of the túngara, for instance, the big, hearty male frog who can take the risk of ending up in a bat stomach is the best possible mate. Risking death is a show of strength and quality, a sign of "good genes." So females evolved to prefer the loud chuck that so appeals to bats.

Dr. Ryan was giving a talk along those lines a dozen years ago and ad libbed. Of course, he said, this idea could be tested by comparing the túngara species, Physalaemus pustulosus, to its near relatives — other species of frog whose mating call is all whines, no chucks.

That comparison surprised him. Dr. Ryan's colleague Dr. Walt Wilczynski, a neuroscientist, established that the hearing systems of the species Physalaemus coloradorum are tuned to be sensitive to the same frequencies as the túngara ear. One inner-ear organ responds to the whine, and the other to the chuck.

If all the ears were tuned alike, might a female of this species actually prefer the túngara chuck to her own male's call? That would be a bit like a woman finding a chimp hoot sexier, say, than "Love Me Tender." Yet that is what happened when the scientists tried it.

So it was not the need for good genes that pushed túngara females to prefer a chuck, Dr. Ryan says. An ancestor's brain and hearing system were wired, for some reason, to like sounds in the chuck range. The túngara females inherited it, as did their cousins in other species. That the preference happens to lead females to larger males, who fertilize more eggs, is a side effect, not a cause.

Dr. Ryan calls this "sensory exploitation" — a new behavior that succeeds in evolution because it takes advantage of existing ways of perceiving the world. The same principle causes a female fish of the swordtail species to prefer males with "swords" extending from their bodies. The swords fit the existing tuning of the fish visual system, inherited from some ancestor. A related species, the platyfish, has no swords. But when a male platyfish has a sword sewn onto his tail, platyfish females prefer him.

To all this, adaptationists answer, essentially, So what? The important principle is that only traits that pass on genes will survive natural selection. How those traits arose is beside the point, argues Dr. Paul W. Sherman, a neurobiologist at Cornell and a leading researcher in the evolution of behavior. He and Dr. Ryan have continuing debates in the journals.

"Regardless of how female preferences and male traits originated," Dr. Sherman wrote in an e-mail message, "they will subsequently co-evolve depending on their reproductive consequences for females."

Though Dr. Sherman says he and Dr. Ryan do not know each other well, Dr. Ryan, characteristically, says they are friends. Nothing seems to ruffle his lively geniality. When he recalls how a big name in neuroscience fiercely demanded at a conference, "If you don't want to do science, why don't you just say so?," he describes the incident in the tones he uses for talking about Austin restaurants or hiking in Panama.

"Yeah, that's what he said," Dr. Ryan said. "Anyway, let me show this computer array. We just awarded a Ph.D. for it yesterday. It lets us analyze every single call that a lab frog makes for its entire life."

NY Times
Back to top
 
WWW  
IP Logged